Gray Matter
What the Brain Can Tell Us About Art
By ERIC R. KANDEL
Published: April 12, 2013
THIS month, President Obama unveiled a breathtakingly ambitious initiative to map the human brain, the ultimate goal of which is to understand the workings of the human mind in biological terms.
Many of the insights that have brought us to this point arose from the
merger over the past 50 years of cognitive psychology, the science of
mind, and neuroscience, the science of the brain. The discipline that
has emerged now seeks to understand the human mind as a set of functions
carried out by the brain.
This new approach to the science of mind not only promises to offer a
deeper understanding of what makes us who we are, but also opens
dialogues with other areas of study — conversations that may help make
science part of our common cultural experience.
Consider what we can learn about the mind by examining how we view figurative art. In a recently published book,
I tried to explore this question by focusing on portraiture, because we
are now beginning to understand how our brains respond to the facial
expressions and bodily postures of others.
The portraiture that flourished in Vienna at the turn of the 20th
century is a good place to start. Not only does this modernist school
hold a prominent place in the history of art, it consists of just three
major artists — Gustav Klimt, Oskar Kokoschka and Egon Schiele — which makes it easier to study in depth.
As a group, these artists sought to depict the unconscious, instinctual
strivings of the people in their portraits, but each painter developed a
distinctive way of using facial expressions and hand and body gestures
to communicate those mental processes.
Their efforts to get at the truth beneath the appearance of an
individual both paralleled and were influenced by similar efforts at the
time in the fields of biology and psychoanalysis. Thus the portraits of
the modernists in the period known as “Vienna 1900” offer a great example of how artistic, psychological and scientific insights can enrich one another.
The idea that truth lies beneath the surface derives from Carl von
Rokitansky, a gifted pathologist who was dean of the Vienna School of
Medicine in the middle of the 19th century. Baron von Rokitansky
compared what his clinician colleague Josef Skoda heard and saw at the
bedsides of his patients with autopsy findings after their deaths. This
systematic correlation of clinical and pathological findings taught them
that only by going deep below the skin could they understand the nature
of illness.
This same notion — that truth is hidden below the surface — was soon steeped in the thinking of Sigmund Freud, who trained at the Vienna School of Medicine
in the Rokitansky era and who used psychoanalysis to delve beneath the
conscious minds of his patients and reveal their inner feelings. That,
too, is what the Austrian modernist painters did in their portraits.
Klimt’s drawings display a nuanced intuition of female sexuality and
convey his understanding of sexuality’s link with aggression, picking up
on things that even Freud missed. Kokoschka and Schiele grasped the
idea that insight into another begins with understanding of oneself. In
honest self-portraits with his lover Alma Mahler, Kokoschka captured
himself as hopelessly anxious, certain that he would be rejected — which
he was. Schiele, the youngest of the group, revealed his vulnerability
more deeply, rendering himself, often nude and exposed, as subject to
the existential crises of modern life.
Such real-world collisions of artistic, medical and biological modes of
thought raise the question: How can art and science be brought together?
Alois Riegl,
of the Vienna School of Art History in 1900, was the first to truly
address this question. He understood that art is incomplete without the
perceptual and emotional involvement of the viewer. Not only does the
viewer collaborate with the artist in transforming a two-dimensional
likeness on a canvas into a three-dimensional depiction of the world,
the viewer interprets what he or she sees on the canvas in personal
terms, thereby adding meaning to the picture. Riegl called this
phenomenon the “beholder’s involvement” or the “beholder’s share.”
Art history was now aligned with psychology. Ernst Kris and Ernst
Gombrich, two of Riegl’s disciples, argued that a work of art is
inherently ambiguous and therefore that each person who sees it has a
different interpretation. In essence, the beholder recapitulates in his
or her own brain the artist’s creative steps.
This insight implied that the brain is a creativity machine, which
obtains incomplete information from the outside world and completes it.
We can see this with illusions and ambiguous figures that trick our
brain into thinking that we see things that are not there. In this
sense, a task of figurative painting is to convince the beholder that an
illusion is true.
Some of this creative process is determined by the way the structure of
our brain develops, which is why we all see the world in pretty much the
same way. However, our brains also have differences that are determined
in part by our individual experiences.
In addition to our built-in visual processes, each of us brings to a
work of art our acquired memories: we remember other works of art that
we have seen. We remember scenes and people that have meaning to us and
relate the work of art to those memories. In order to see what is
painted on a canvas, we have to know beforehand what we might see in a
painting. These insights into perception served as a bridge between the
visual perception of art and the biology of the brain.
So how does our brain respond to portraiture? As we look at a portrait,
our brain calls on several interacting systems to analyze contours, form
a representation of the face and of the body, analyze the body’s
motion, experience emotion, and perhaps, empathy. Along with these
instantaneous responses, we form a theory of the subject’s state of
mind.
The brain’s representation of faces is especially important to the
beholder’s response to portraiture. Our brain devotes more space to
reading the details of faces than to any other object. We react strongly
to the expressionist works of these Viennese artists, in part, because
our brain contains specialized cells that respond powerfully to the
exaggerated facial features these painters portrayed.
Moreover, the sense of stimulation we often experience when we look at a
portrait is thought to be due in part to the activity of “mirror
neurons.” Signaling by these cells in the motor areas of the brain can
make us perceive the actions of others as if they were our own.
All of which goes to show that the real “eye” of the beholder is the brain itself.